Search
On-Site Program Calendar
Browse By Day
Browse By Time
Browse By Person
Browse By Room
Browse By Unit
Browse By Session Type
Search Tips
Annual Meeting Housing and Travel
Sign In
X (Twitter)
Objectives. A child’s math self-concept is a powerful predictor of their math achievement and choices (Marsh et al., 2005). Math self-concept typically declines between elementary and high school (Cvencek et al., 2021), and this decline is stronger for girls than boys (Sáinz & Eccles, 2011). Cross-sectional studies provide a possible explanation for the larger decline in girls’ self-concept. The societal stereotypes that “boys are better in math” may impede girls from developing beliefs in their math competence during early adolescence (Cvencek et al., 2021; Sáinz & Eccles, 2012). However, it is important to longitudinally examine the link between girls’ gender-ability stereotypes and math self-concept to examine how these stereotypes influence the development of math self-concepts over time (Parker et al., 2015). Our primary aim was to examine how math gender-ability stereotypes influenced girls’ math self-concepts across multiple school levels (elementary and middle/high school) from a longitudinal perspective.
Theoretical Framework. As a theoretical framework, we use Marsh and colleagues’ (2005) reciprocal effects model of academic self-concept, which examines how self-concepts and achievement influence one another over time in the context of societal stereotypes.
Method. The current sample included N=422 students (7–14 years old at Time 1, Mean=11.49, SD=1.98; 48% girls) who completed an online survey three times over a 12-month period. Math self-concepts were measured with two items (αs=.87–.89, e.g., “I am good at math activities,” 1-6 scale). Stereotypes were measured with a difference score, “how good are most boys at math” minus “how good are most girls,” 1-6 scale; difference score range -5 to 5. We conducted a two-level linear growth model using Stata15.0 to analyze a data structure where students (level-2) were repeatedly measured across three time-points (level-1). Model testing proceeded in three phases: the unconditional model, the unconditional growth model, and the conditional model (see Supplementary Material).
Results. Results are shown in Table 1. First, average change in math self-concept was not significant across time. Second, the significant three-way interaction of gender×school-level×gender-ability stereotypes across time indicated that gender and school-level significantly moderated the influence of gender-ability stereotypes on the trajectory of students’ math self-concepts. Breaking down this interaction, we examined the simple effect of time on math self-concept among girls who endorsed ability stereotypes favoring boys or favoring girls at different school levels (elementary- and middle/high-school). The only significant decrease in math self-concepts over time was found among elementary-school girls who endorsed stereotypes favoring boys (Figure 1).
Significance. This study examined the influence of gender-ability stereotypes on math self-concept among students in elementary and secondary schools across a 12-month period. We found a significant association between girls’ math gender-ability stereotypes and self-concept longitudinally. Elementary-school girls who endorsed ability stereotypes that favored boys were more likely to have a decreasing trajectory in math self-concept. These findings demonstrate how gender stereotypes and biases can have a negative impact on young girls’ math self-concept development. Taken together, our study highlights the need to protect girls’ self-concepts from negative gender stereotypes about their abilities and promote their motivation in math.